Printer Friendly

Examining climate change awareness and climate-friendly activities of urban residents: A case study in Kosice.

Introduction

Cities and urban residents have a crucial role to play in the development of climate change mitigation and adaptation. There are estimates that 50% to 80% of the measures which are necessary to mitigate the impacts of climate change require regional and local implementation and also suggest that the decisions of local governments can potentially influence up to one-third of all urban greenhouse gas emissions (OECD, 2021). The necessary public support of climate change policies is affected by people's environmental beliefs (Kacha et al., 2022). As a result, cities should make efforts to systematically raise awareness and involve governance factors as a standard procedure in adaptation efforts (Tapia et al., 2017). In order to do so, local policymakers must be well informed about the preferences of residents, their motivation and engagement in climate-friendly activities.

The aim of this paper is to examine the factors affecting engagement in adaptation and mitigation activities among individuals in the urban environment. Although cities are considered among the key actors of climate action, the existing literature on climate change awareness and action has primarily examined the issue at the national level or among specific socio-economic groups (Valenzuela-Levi et al., 2022). Moreover, little attention has been paid to specifically urban topics, such as the impact of heatwaves (Lenzholzer et al., 2020), which will be the greatest climate-related issue in many European cities. Additionally, there is a distinct lack of studies which have investigated urban awareness of climate change within the specific geographical context of Central and Eastern Europe. Individual nations and population groups often hold idiosyncratic views on climate change (Kacha et al., 2022), and regions embedded in various socio-economic and cultural milieus could form differing associations to climate change (Poortinga et al., 2019). Our study thus contributes to the existing literature by providing a novel perspective on climate change awareness and engagement in a local urban context.

Our study focuses on climate change awareness among the population of the city of Kosice in Slovakia. We chose this city due to the fact that Central Europe, and especially urban areas, were under-represented in the previous research. The context of the post-socialist city with historically low citizen engagement might offer valuable insights into why some areas are lagging behind with their adaptation efforts. In addition, Kosice is a city with a prominent industrial heritage, which also indirectly influences peoples' views on green transformation and sustainable futures. Being in this specific situation, the mechanism of climate change awareness and citizen engagement in climate friendly action might take on some specific context-dependent form.

Our case study offers an analysis of climate change perceptions and the adaptation and mitigation activities of city residents, the findings of which could serve as a first step in developing an effective communication strategy between authorities and local citizens. In the paper, we address the following questions. What factors influence the awareness of urban residents regarding climate change? What factors influence local citizens' engagement in climate-friendly activities? What is the connection between awareness and action? Do the factors differ depending on the cost of implementing the activities?

The paper is organized as follows: Section 1 offers a general description of the context and a literature review discussing the factors which influence attitudes to climate change, followed by a description of the data and methodology in Section 2. Section 3 applies regression analysis and the random forests model to analyze the data and presents a discussion of the main findings of the research, while the Anal section concludes with possible policy implications.

1. Theoretical background

1.1 Factors affecting climate change awareness and climate-friendly activities

Levels of climate change awareness have been found to differ across countries, contexts and individuals. A review by Gifford and Nilsson (2014) and a meta-analysis by Hornsey et al. (2016) offer a comprehensive list of the relevant factors which determine climate change beliefs and attitudes. The most intuitive characteristics relevant to climate change perceptions are the more sociodemographic aspects such as gender, age and education. People who accept the reality of climate change are usually younger, more educated and from higher income brackets, with the categories of gender or race having a less significant impact on awareness. Climate change awareness also differs across countries (Poortinga et al., 2019). While a belief that human activities are contributing to climate change is a predictor of risk perceptions in Latin America and Europe, in Asia and Africa, changes in temperature are seen as the most prominent (Lee et al., 2015). Even the effect of education is not uniform across countries or among different political affiliations of citizens (Czarnek et al., 2021). In more developed countries, the topic of climate change is seen as a more politicized topic, and in such a context, education alone might not be enough to raise awareness about the risks of climate change, especially among right-wing voters (ibid). Differences were also identified between European countries, with the effects of demographic and socio-political factors found to be less significant in Eastern European countries than in Western states (Poortinga et al., 2019).

In addition to the sociodemographic and country-specific contexts, other factors that could be classified as psychological have been identified. The levels of subjective knowledge of scientific findings concerning climate change were largely the same for both "believers" and "sceptics," but a higher level of objectively measured knowledge is connected to a stronger belief in the reality of climate change (Hornsey et al., 2016). Personal traits such as open-mindedness, conscientiousness and lower emotional stability were also found to be related to environmental concerns (Gifford & Nilsson, 2014). Moreover, a willingness to support climate-related policies also stems from individuals' attitudes towards long-term planning or attachment to place (Allo & Loureiro, 2014), with those expressing a stronger attachment to global rather than national identities being more likely aware of climate change and hold a positive view towards climate change responses (Devine-Wright et al., 2015).

In terms of the magnitude of the effects, the socio-economic and psychological characteristics were overshadowed by variables connected to values, beliefs, political affiliation, worldviews and culture (Hornsey et al., 2016). One of the most important factors is that of social identity (ibid); individuals who identified with a "green" or activist identity or who stated that they valued the natural environment were more likely to believe in the reality of climate change.

Theories of risk perception tend to suggest that personal experience would be expected to affect perceived risk and that the personal relevance of the issue would have a greater effect than a reliance of cognitive information (Howe et al., 2014). As was shown in a meta-analysis by Allo and Loureiro (2014), a direct experience of extreme weather events increases the willingness to accept the costs of mitigation and adaptation policies. Other empirical research has shown that although personal experience with extreme weather conditions and their impacts is connected to a stronger belief in climate change, this association is not considered to be particularly significant (Hornsey et al., 2016). Sometimes, even individuals who have had direct experience with extreme weather events are not necessarily convinced of the need to adopt policies directed at climate change mitigation and adaptation (Gartner & Schoen, 2021). Interestingly, this relationship also functions inversely, as our perception of our experiences can be influenced by our beliefs, with individuals' opinions about global warming influencing their likelihood of recollecting extreme weather events (Kacha et al., 2022). A feedback mechanism between climate change perceptions and negative effects has also been identified, which suggests that people process cognitive information and affect heuristics simultaneously (Linden, 2014). A perceived susceptibility to climate change was found to be connected to a greater willingness to engage in mitigation activities (Semenza et al., 2011). The phenomenon of self-reported heat stress in urban residents and its associations with coping strategies was the subject of a study by Kunz-Plapp et al. (2016) but the results were inconclusive. This is why more research focused on the relationship between vulnerability and action in urban environment is needed.

1.2 The awareness-action gap

Earlier research into translating perceptions to actions has shown that belief in climate change alone is not a sufficient predictor of the adoption of environmentally friendly activities; indeed, studies suggest that belief is more connected with an intention to act than with actual activity (Hornsey et al., 2016). Perhaps the most methodologically comprehensive and statistically robust work carried out on the topic to date is the paper by Saari et al. (2021), which illustrates that environmental risk perception and environmental knowledge, mediated by environmental concerns, can be translated into behavioral intention and realization.

In general, one of the more perplexing findings of research into this issue is the complexity of the translation mechanisms from awareness and action, with some studies even refuting the idea that there is a direct link between awareness and action. A number of studies have already shown that awareness of climate change and its impacts offers no accurate prediction of the likelihood of adopting climate-friendly activities, an effect which has been termed the "awareness-action gap" (Csutora, 2012) or the attitude-behavior gap (Farjam et al., 2019). This disparity between different components of environmental awareness is a consequence of the complex nature of reality and economic-structural factors (Csutora, 2012), such as the embeddedness of individual behavior within social and institutional contexts (Jackson, 2005) or the unwillingness of consumers to relinquish unsustainable lifestyles perpetuated by social norms (Sanne, 2002).

1.3 Economic aspects of climate-friendly activities

Previous studies have suggested that varying costs of climate-relevant behavior can have an impact on the adoption of such measures. Some theoretical background is offered in the low-cost hypothesis, which postulates that behavioral costs can influence the effects of attitudes on behavior (Diekmann & Preisendorfer, 2003). In addition to the financial aspects, costs can be viewed in terms of time, discomfort or effort expenditure. According to Diekmann and Preisendorfer (2003), environmental concerns tend to influence ecological behavior to a greater extent when associated with lower costs and inconvenience. Attitudes are more likely to translate into corresponding behavior when the actions are uncomplicated and affordable, but environmental concern alone is insufficient to overcome barriers associated with behaviors that entail high costs or considerable levels of inconvenience (ibid). In such cases, education and financial resources might mediate the connection between belief and action (Stern, 1992). Engagement in low-cost climate-friendly behaviors has been shown to be positively related to factors such as age, levels of concern and the perception of climate benefits (Tobler et al., 2012). In the case of high-cost activities, government bodies could play a role in introducing incentives and cost-reducing measures to motivate climate-friendly actions (Jakucionyte-Skodiene & Liobikiene, 2022).

A correlation has been identified between climate action and environmental injustice (Castan Broto & Westman, 2020). Individuals with lower financial resources encounter barriers in participating in climate-friendly actions as they typically lack the means to invest in expensive measures but also lack access to more affordable activities. In light of this, research into local climate adaptation action has shifted towards poverty-alleviation agendas which are focused on addressing socio-economic disparities and the issue of marginalization. The aim is to examine ways of fostering deeper inclusion and equity within urban climate governance.

1.4 Case study background: Kosice

Based on a recent study conducted by the OECD (2023) and the Institute of Environmental Policy of the Slovak Ministry of Environment containing an assessment based on 10 levels of risk, Kosice in particular, has been identified as a high-risk district, primarily due to its higher risk of drought (level 8) and extreme heat waves (level 6). In contrast, the risk of extreme precipitation in Kosice is relatively low, rated at level 3. Extreme heat was also identified as the most salient climate-change related issue in Kosice in studies from the European Environmental Agency (European Environment Agency, 2020), which compiled a list of the observed and projected impacts for seven types of biogeographical regions in the EU. When it comes to conditions for climate action, there is an industrial heritage connected to the still active steel industry and several disused industrial brownfield sites, an environmental burden which poses a challenge (or an opportunity) for future adaptation policies.

In 2022, Kosice introduced its first adaptation plan for climate change for 2022-2030, which builds upon vulnerability assessments and recommends specific goals in the climate change adaptation process. The effort to pursue a more sustainable future is also manifested in the city's application for the European Green Capital Award and its participation in the 100 Climate Neutral Cities 2030 initiative. Since 2019, Kosice has also been part of the Covenant of Mayors for Climate and Energy initiative, pledging to reduce GHG emissions and to increase climate change resilience through adaptation strategies. One distinctive aspect of the city lies in the limited engagement of local residents in political participation, a trend which is particularly evident in data concerning local elections. In 2022, Kosice recorded the lowest voter turnout for local self-government and mayoral elections when compared to eight cities of similar size and nature. Only 31% of residents participated in the local elections, whereas the average participation rate across the other eight cities stood at 41.2%, with a median of 39.3% (Statistical Office of the Slovak Republic, 2022).

2. Research methodology

The data analyzed in this paper originates from a survey which was primarily conducted in an online format among the residents of Kosice from October 2019 to January 2020. The questionnaire was accessible through social media, the web page of the KOSICE [+ or -] 40 project and the web pages of the partners of the project, and information about the questionnaire was also sent by the municipal authorities to all affiliated municipal institutions with the request that it be distributed among their employees. The questionnaire was also distributed to 100 businesses in Kosice and was made available in physical format for use in schools and retirement homes. For the purposes of this analysis, only the responses received from Kosice residents were considered, with the total dataset consisting of 545 valid responses.

The questionnaire contained questions concerning sociodemographic characteristics such as age, income, education, sex, household size and overall levels of satisfaction. The main focus was placed on attitudes towards climate change, respondents' personal experience with its impact, satisfaction with the availability of information on the topic and measures concerning climate change impacts. The study also aimed to identify the types of climate-friendly activities in which local residents engaged and to gauge their participation in local politics.

Tab. 1 lists a statistical breakdown of the questionnaire respondents. From a demographic perspective, the age of respondents ranged from 15 to 87, with both a mean and median age of 40. Most of the respondents stated that they belonged to the middle-income group. There were slightly more women than men, and most of the respondents had children. Participants stated that they were generally satisfied with their lives. As the survey was voluntary, most of the respondents had a pre-existing interest in the topic of climate change, and this resulted in a potentially biased sample with an overrepresentation of people with a stronger interest in the topic.

The focus of our analysis was placed on gaining an understanding of the factors that impact awareness and engagement in climate-friendly activities. This was achieved by formulating nine climate-related variables which were subsequently examined in our models; these variables are listed in Tab. 2.

The variables were examined using the sociodemographic indicators as control variables. The variables were mostly created in the form of indices compiled from the Likert scale responses to the statements provided on the topic.

Tab. 3 shows the categorization of climate-friendly activities based on their associated costs. It lists all provided climate-friendly activities divided into two categories, based on which dependent variables were created. These groups were assigned intuitively --low-cost activities are mostly behavioral adjustments, such as following extreme weather alerts or sorting the waste for recycling. High-cost activities include examples that are associated with certain material and/or financial resources, such as having air conditioners.

In order to analyze the data, we first ran a standard linear model examining the associations between the dependent and explanatory variables while monitoring the effect of the socio-economic factors. We created four OLS models with different dependent variables. The models were checked for multicollinearity and the results were negative. If any heteroskedasticity was detected, robust standard errors were reported. The four regression models were specified as follows (Equations (1-4)):

Model 1:

[Awareness.sub.i] = [[beta].sub.0] + [[beta].sub.1] * [sociodem.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.2] * [Interest.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.3] * [Vulnerability.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.4] * [Engagement.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.5] * [Information.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.6] * [Activities.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.7] * [Measures.sub.i] + [[epsilon].sub.i] (1)

Model 2:

[Activities.sub.i] = [[beta].sub.0] + [[beta].sub.1] * [sociodem.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.2] * [Interest.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.3] * [Vulnerability.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.4] * [Engagement.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.5] * [Information.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.6] * [Measures.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.7] * [Awareness.sub.i] + [[epsilon].sub.i] (2)

Model 3:

[Activities(cheap).sub.i] = [[beta].sub.0] + [[beta].sub.1] * [sociodem.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.2] * [Interest.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.3] * [Vulnerability.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.4] * [Engagement.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.5] * [Information.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.6] * [Measures.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.7] * [Awareness.sub.i] + [[epsilon].sub.i] (3)

Model 4:

[Activities(expensive).sub.i] = [[beta].sub.0] + [[beta].sub.1] * [sociodem.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.2] * [Interest.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.3] * [Vulnerability.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.4] * [Engagement.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.5] * [Information.sub.i] + + [[beta].sub.6] * [Measures.sub.i] + [[beta].sub.7] * [Awareness.sub.i] + [[epsilon].sub.i] (4)

where: sociodem--the set of sociodemographic variables described in Tab. 1; Awareness, Interest, Vulnerability, Engagement, Information, Activities, Measures--the variables described in the Tab. 2 and Tab. 3; [[beta].sub.0]--the regression intercept; [[beta].sub.1-7]--coefficients corresponding to the explanatory variables; [epsilon]--error.

The second methodological approach applied an exploratory technique using a machine learning algorithm which was used to validate the regression results and indicate non-linear relations between the investigated concepts. The random forest (RF) technique developed by Breiman (2001) was applied in this process. Our study adopted a bagging algorithm using the "randomForest" R Package based on Breiman and Cutler's random forests for classification and regression. The parameters were set as follows: first, the number of trees to grow was set to 500, and the number of variables randomly sampled as candidates at each split was set to 5. After running the model, the out-of-bag mean squared error stabilized at around 150 trees and there was no additional gain in increasing the number of trees. This method was applied in order to identify any potential hidden relationships that could not be described by the standard linear model. This was intended as an exploratory step to either confirm the results of the previous models or to suggest further improvements in potential future research. As there was no intention to make inference statements, it was not considered necessary to divide the data into the training and testing sets. Similar machine learning algorithms have been used in other studies, e.g., by Lee et al. (2015), which examined the predictors of climate change awareness and risk perceptions in different countries. The study selected this method due to its high predictive accuracy and capacity to provide an unbiased and robust ranking of predictor importance and account for complex interactions between predictors and unbalanced response classes while preserving as much information in the data as possible. The analysis also avoided the need to divide the sample into training and testing sets, as the prediction accuracy for each model was calculated using a built-in out-of-bag test sample provided in the R function of the package (this option was also selected in our analysis).

3. Results and discussion

3.1 Results

The results from the first model examining factors associated with climate change awareness revealed several key findings. The most significant explanatory variable was subjective vulnerability, indicating that individuals who perceived themselves as vulnerable to the impacts of climate change were more likely to be aware of the issue. Additionally, interest in the topic of climate change emerged as an important factor, suggesting that individuals with a general interest in climate change were more likely to be aware of its implications. The results also indicated a negative relationship between age and awareness, indicating that younger people tended to be more concerned about climate change. Overall, the model accounted for 27% of the variance in the dependent variable. The findings indicate that individuals who are aware of climate change are typically younger, have an existing interest in the topic, and have personally experienced the effects of climate change in their everyday lives.

Several significant predictors were identified in the second model, which explored the factors associated with engagement in climate-friendly activities encompassing both mitigation and adaptation measures. Citizen engagement emerged as the most effective predictor, suggesting that individuals who actively participated in climate-friendly actions were more likely to engage in such activities. Subjective vulnerability also played a significant role, indicating that those who perceived themselves as vulnerable to climate change were more likely to engage in climate-friendly actions. Age and satisfaction with the information provided on climate change and adaptation strategies were positively associated with the dependent variables, suggesting that older individuals and those who felt adequately informed were more likely to engage in climate-friendly activities. Gender was also a relevant factor in this model, with male respondents displaying a more negative association, although the significance level was relatively low. Overall, the model accounted for 30% of the variance in the dependent variable. The results from this model suggest that individuals who engage in climate-friendly activities are more likely to be women who are active citizens with personal experience of the negative effects of climate change and feel that they have sufficient information on adaptation measures. Additionally, the level of engagement tended to increase slightly with age.

The third model focused on climate-friendly activities that are inexpensive or cost-free. Similar to the previous models, citizen engagement emerged as a significant factor, indicating that individuals who actively participate in these low-cost activities were more likely to engage in climate-friendly behaviors. Vulnerability and satisfaction with information also played relevant roles, suggesting that individuals who perceived themselves as vulnerable to climate change impacts and felt adequately informed were more likely to engage in such activities. Gender showed a high significance in this context, although the magnitude of the effect was not substantial. The model accounted for approximately 30% of the variance in the data. This model suggests that individuals engaging in inexpensive climate-friendly activities are likely to be women who are also involved in local politics and do not perceive a lack of available information on adaptation measures.

The predictive power of vulnerability and citizen engagement remained high in the fourth model, which examined expensive climate-friendly activities, but a new variable emerged as relevant: the parental status of respondents. Individuals with children were found to have a higher likelihood of engaging in expensive climate-friendly activities. Additionally, higher incomes and advanced age were associated with an increased likelihood of participating in these activities.

Overall, the models did not identify a high proportion of variance in the dependent variable, as the adjusted R-squared rate was between 0.2-0.3. However, it should be noted that this is a common occurrence in social science research, and similar studies achieve comparable values in this metric (e.g., Rosentrater et al., 2013; Shi et al., 2015).

The Anal part of the analysis examined possible non-linear relationships between the dependent and explanatory variables. The direction of the relationships was not the main focus of this analysis as this characteristic cannot be identified using the random forests algorithm. The random forests model can be interpreted using feature importance values which are relative in the model and are therefore not numerically significant. These values are used to rank the input variables that influence the target variable. The random forests algorithm allows us to identify the variables most responsible for decreasing variance through node purity, thereby determining the most important features for predicting the dependent variable. The derived feature importance is shown in Fig. 1a-1d as IncNodePurity, a value representing the total decrease in node impurities measured by the Gini index. The expressed measure relates to the decline in purity when a specific variable lacks information. In scenarios where a variable possesses no initial information, the resultant decline would amount to zero.

The first random forests model examines the predictors associated with "awareness" (Fig. 1a). The most important independent variable is that of self-assessed vulnerability, followed by age and satisfaction with measures. Among the least important variables are sociodemographic characteristics "children," "sex," "education" and "income."

The results of the second model (Fig. 1b) confirmed that citizen engagement in local politics ("engagement"), vulnerability and age might help explain the variance of climate-friendly actions. Satisfaction with local adaptation measures (the "measures" variable) was also found to be a good predictor of climate-friendly action. In contrast to the findings of the regression results, awareness was only the fourth most important variable, although it is at a comparable level of importance with age and satisfaction. This suggests that awareness might still matter but that the relationship cannot be described by a linear model. With the exception of age, the sociodemographic variables were assessed as having low predictive power.

The two models investigating the predictors of engagement in the case of low and high cost climate-friendly activities differ slightly in their results (Fig. 1c-1d). Corroborating the results from the OLS, engagement in politics was found to play a crucial role in explaining engagement in cheaper climate-friendly activities. Subjective vulnerability, awareness and satisfaction with measures showed broadly similar levels of importance. Employing expensive measures was associated with vulnerability and age, followed by satisfaction with measures, awareness and engagement in local politics. The low-cost hypothesis could not be reliably confirmed by the above models, as awareness ranked among the most important variables for both low and high cost activities. As with the regression results, age plays a more important role in explaining engagement in high-cost activities rather than cheaper measures. This could be due to the straightforward correlation of age and wealth rather than income, a value which was controlled for in the analysis.

3.2 Discussion

Our analysis not only confirms the previous findings on the factors affecting climate change awareness and action in general, but also sheds light on the topic in the under-researched context of the urban environment and relates it to engagement in local politics. The significance of sociodemographic attributes, notably age, sex, income and parental status outlined, e.g., in Gifford and Nilsson (2014) or in Hornsey et al. (2016), was validated across various model specifications. The role of age was evaluated as highly important in the non-linear models but as somewhat less salient in the linear analyses. This interpretation is in agreement with earlier research into the prediction of climate change awareness, which also indicated that awareness was more profound in the case of younger people (Hornsey et al., 2016). The nature of the results relating to the prediction of action is even clearer when activities are divided in terms of cost, with age becoming positively associated with a higher probability of engaging in more expensive activities. This interpretation is intuitive when we examine the specific activities that were included in the survey, such as making home improvements, choosing vacation destinations, or buying a new car, activities that are naturally more typically associated with later adulthood than with young age. Furthermore, the positive coefficients associated with income and having children which were noted in the case of expensive activities, are also intuitive. These characteristics were only found to be relevant in this one model, which suggests that parents with higher incomes have the means and/or the motivation to engage in more costly adaptation-related activities. In the previous literature, income was found to be positively correlated with climate change beliefs (Hornsey et al., 2016).

One aspect of the findings which differed from those of earlier studies was the level of education; this factor was not found to be relevant in any of the models in our study. This missing link perhaps deserves further research that considers the specific conditions of different education systems. The findings suggest that in Slovakia, a post-socialist country which still enjoys a significant degree of egalitarianism in its education system, the influence of education may be less significant than in other countries. Or, as suggested by Czarnek et al. (2021), the effect of education could be dependent on the political ideology and/or the level of the country's development. This could mean that Slovakia is among the developed countries, where the positive effect of education is attenuated by the right-wing ideology.

On the other hand, our research did confirm the connection between sex and engagement in climate action. We found that women were more likely to take part in low-cost mitigation and adaptation activities, a finding which could be explained by earlier research which has associated the different stances towards adaptation to climate change among male and female respondents to the factor of motivation --men are more likely to be motivated by financial reasons and advances in technology, while women tend to be more egalitarian and prioritize the community and ecological aspects of measures (Allo & Loureiro, 2014; Brink & Wamsler, 2019).

In the case of psychological factors, we found that personal experience with negative impacts of climate change in everyday life is of great importance in both awareness and all types of activities. In the literature, this relationship has been found to vary, with some studies postulating that experience increases support for climate policies (Allo & Loureiro, 2014), while others finding that some people do not And their experience convincing of the need to adopt policies directed at climate change mitigation and adaptation (Gartner & Schoen, 2021). In addition, a cognitive aspect to the awareness-action gap was identified. This is in line with the hypothesis that personal experience is also responsible for shaping human behavior rather than cognitive information alone. This means that in order to become fully aware and willing to take action, urban residents need to internalize not only their knowledge of the issue, but also the connection between their negative experiences and the reality of climate change. In addition, the availability of information about how to adapt to climate change has been found to motivate low-cost behavior. This finding is in line with those reported by Shi et al. (2015) which identified a nexus between action-related knowledge (in our case, information about possible ways of adapting) and a willingness to adjust behaviors.

In practice, the findings of the study are of great importance for policymakers' communication strategies. The information that stresses the direct relationship between climate change and peoples' exposure to the consequences of heatwaves, flash floods, or drought will be the most efficient means of conveying the desired message and motivating action among local populations. Indeed, by providing guidance on how to mitigate the impacts of climate-related changes, local authorities could also foster further activities. However, in order to gain support for climate change policies, the authorities should communicate through causal knowledge and avoid interfering with peoples' cultural values (Shi et al., 2015). In addition, result-oriented communication focusing on the uncontrollable consequences of climate change should be avoided, as this could engender feelings of resignation and helplessness (ibid).

Another important aspect to consider when designing policies and communication strategy is the economic cost of climate-related adaptation and mitigation activities. This aspect of the issue increases in importance when we take into account the urgency of costly adaptation measures. Although we found that the availability of information on adaptation measures might help to motivate engagement in low-cost activities, it is imperative to consider the potential downsides of such an approach, with engagement in low-cost behaviors potentially undermining more expansive efforts to mitigate climate change through costly yet effective action (Hagmann et al., 2019). Environmentally concerned individuals reduce the cognitive dissonance between their attitudes and the impacts of their actions through low-cost behaviors, which subsequently lessens their need to engage in costly behaviors. Attempts to offer quick fixes to complex problems such as climate change always have the potential to backfire and result in unforeseen consequences (ibid).

Additionally, the connection to climate justice is an important aspect to consider in the interpretation of the results. Climate justice emphasizes the equitable distribution of the burdens and benefits of climate change and the fair participation of all individuals and communities in decision-making processes. In the context of local climate governance, it is crucial to assess whether certain demographic groups, particularly those who may be more vulnerable to the impact of climate change, have equitable access to information, resources, and opportunities for engagement in climate-friendly activities. Our case study indicated lower levels of involvement among older sections of the population.

Earlier studies have emphasized the importance of identity (Hornsey et al., 2016), and this can also be implied from our results concerning the impact of engagement in local politics. Active participation in local political and civic life greatly increases the likelihood of engagement in climate-friendly activities. We can also suggest the possible existence of an "activist" identity that, although it could not be measured directly in the survey, can still be discerned in the observed data. When designing measures, local policymakers can capitalize on the motivation of these already active citizens, who can serve as inspirational role models for their communities. Further research on climate perceptions and climate-friendly behavior in cities could further explore how to build upon the connection between attachment to place and action. Promotional activities related to building a city's brand and identity could potentially enhance local citizens' engagement in climate-friendly activities. Additionally, this could suggest that supporting civil society represents an indirect contribution to the fostering of adaptive activities. Allocating resources to community-building initiatives and promoting participatory climate governance is particularly effective in cities with lower levels of citizen engagement, such as Kosice.

There are limitations connected to the sample and the sampling method employed in the survey. Although our data is derived from a relatively large sample of questionnaire responses, the sample was potentially biased as many of the respondents to the questionnaire showed a pre-existing interest in the topic of climate change. People with higher educational attainments were also overrepresented in the sample, and there were relatively few respondents from low-income brackets. As a result, future research should ensure that respondents are selected irrespective of their interest in the topic and should also make an effort to include marginalized groups. Previous research has also shown that awareness and action can be heavily influenced by pre-existing values and identities, and therefore, surveys which aim to map climate change perceptions should also attempt to collect responses on these topics in order to ensure a more balanced dataset.

Conclusions

The identification of the co-founding factors of climate change awareness and subsequent action represents a crucial step in understanding the mechanisms which should be employed in order to ensure the successful adoption of adaptation and mitigation policies across different demographic groups. Our study not only confirms the general principles identified in earlier research but also offers a specific context related to urban residents and local politics. The main contribution to the existing literature is twofold--first, by accounting for the local context, we addressed the debate on climate change adaptation at the most important level --cities. Second, by choosing Kosice, we provided a case study of the under-researched geographical area, which has its specific conditions.

As for the comparison with the existing research, the analysis has confirmed multiple sociodemographic characteristics outlined in e.g., Hornsey et al. (2016) as having an impact on either awareness or engagement in climate-friendly activities. Those with higher awareness levels tend to be younger and those more engaged are usually women. The missing link between education and climate beliefs can be explained by politization of the topic in developed countries (Czarnek et al., 2021). Our findings on the role of subjective vulnerability contributes to conflicting results of the previous researchers, e.g., Kunz-Plapp et al. (2016) or Gartner and Schoen (2021), indicating that own experience with negative impacts might indeed motivate people to act. When it comes to the previous research on the economics aspects, attitudes were found to be more likely to translate into corresponding behavior when the actions were uncomplicated and affordable, but environmental concern alone was found to be insufficient to overcome barriers associated with behaviors that entail high costs or considerable levels of inconvenience (Diekmann & Preisendorfer, 2003). Our analysis could not completely confirm this (due to borderline low significance level), but we found that having information matters when it comes to low-cost measures.

The results of the study draw attention to several policy implications. As the findings suggest that cognitive awareness alone might not be sufficient to motivate people to act, awareness-raising campaigns should not necessarily be seen as the most effective means of achieving higher levels of engagement and it might be more effective to confront people with the impacts of climate change through their own experience and vulnerability. As a result, approaches which place a greater emphasis on the direct connection between climate change and its immediate manifestations in the local environment and on peoples' everyday lives could potentially motivate local residents to become more actively involved in mitigation and adaptation activities. As previous studies have suggested, the availability of action-related information was found to affect individuals' willingness to act. This means that communication strategies should also include recommendations for specific climate-friendly activities and their potential effect.

We also identified a connection between climate-friendly action and participation in local politics, but we do not view this nexus as a causal relationship but rather as co-occurring phenomena, a correlation which could be a manifestation of an as-yet undetermined activist identity. From this perspective, participatory community-building activities organized by local stakeholders ranging from local authorities to NGOs or other actors could encourage those who hesitate to engage on their own, an approach which might be more effective than campaigns focused on raising awareness. The sense of identity engendered by belonging to a group with an activist identity can also promote citizen engagement in a broader sense.

https://doi.org/10.15240/tul/001/2024-1-002

Acknowledgments: This paper was supported by VEGA Research Grant No. 1/0681/22 and Research Grant No. APVV-19-0263.

References

Allo, M., & Loureiro, M. L. (2014). The role of social norms on preferences towards climate change policies: A meta-analysis. Energy Policy, 73, 563-574. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.enpol.2014.04.042

Breiman, L. (2001). Random forests. Machine Learning, 45(1), 5-32. https://doi.org/ 10.1023/a:1010933404324

Brink, E., & Wamsler, C. (2019). Citizen engagement in climate adaptation surveyed: The role of values, worldviews, gender and place. Journal of Cleaner Production, 209, 1342-1353. https://doi.org/10.1016/jJclepro.2018.10.164

Castan Broto, V., & Westman, L. K. (2020). Ten years after Copenhagen: Reimagining climate change governance in urban areas. WIREs Climate Change, 11 (4). https://doi. org/10.1002/wcc.643

Csutora, M. (2012). One more awareness gap? The behaviour-impact gap problem. Journal of Consumer Policy, 35(1), 145-163. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10603-012-9187-8

Czarnek, G., Kossowska, M., & Szwed, P. (2021). Right-wing ideology reduces the effects of education on climate change beliefs in more developed countries. Nature Climate Change, 11(1), 9-13. https://doi.org/10.1038/ s41558-020-00930-6

Devine-Wright, P., Price, J., & Leviston, Z. (2015). My country or my planet? Exploring the influence of multiple place attachments and ideological beliefs upon climate change attitudes and opinions. Global Environmental Change, 30, 68-79. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.gloenvcha.2014.10.012

Diekmann, A., & Preisendorfer, P. (2003). Green and greenback: The behavioral effects of environmental attitudes in low-cost and high-cost situations. Rationality and Society, 15(4), 441-472. https://doi.org/10.1177/1043463103154002

European Environment Agency. (2020). Urban adaptation in Europe: How cities and towns respond to climate change. Publications Office of the European Union. https://data.europa.eu/ doi/10.2800/324620

Farjam, M., Nikolaychuk, O., & Bravo, G. (2019). Experimental evidence of an environmental attitude-behavior gap in high-cost situations. Ecological Economics, 166, 106434. https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.ecolecon.2019.106434

Gartner, L., & Schoen, H. (2021). Experiencing climate change: Revisiting the role of local weather in affecting climate change awareness and related policy preferences. Climatic Change, 167(3), 31. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-02103176-z

Gifford, R., & Nilsson, A. (2014). Personal and social factors that influence pro-environmental concern and behaviour: A review. International Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 141-157. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12034

Hagmann, D., Ho, E. H., & Loewenstein, G. (2019). Nudging out support for a carbon tax. Nature Climate Change, 9(6), 484-489. https:// doi.org/10.1038/s41558-019-0474-0

Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., Bain, P. G., & Fielding, K. S. (2016). Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nature Climate Change, 6(6), 622-626. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2943

Howe, P. D., Boudet, H., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. W. (2014). Mapping the shadow of experience of extreme weather events. Climatic Change, 127(2), 381-389. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10584-014-1253-6

Jackson, T (2005). Motivating sustainable consumption: A review of evidence on consumer behaviour and behavioural change [Report]. Sustainable Development Research Network. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/ 275638627_Motivating_Sustainable_ Consumption_A_Review_of_Evidence_on_Consumer_Behaviour_and_Behavioural_Change

Jakucionyte-Skodiene, M., & Liobikiene, G. (2022). The changes in climate change concern, responsibility assumption and impact on climate-friendly behaviour in EU from the Paris Agreement until 2019. Environmental Management, 69(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00267-021-01574-8

Kacha, O., Vintr, J., & Brick, C. (2022). Four Europes: Climate change beliefs and attitudes predict behavior and policy preferences using a latent class analysis on 23 countries. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 81, 101815. https://doi.org/10.1016/jJenvp.2022.101815

Kunz-Plapp, T., Hackenbruch, J., & Schipper, J. W (2016). Factors of subjective heat stress of urban citizens in contexts of everyday life. Natural Hazards and Earth System Sciences, 16(4), 977-994. https://doi.org/10.5194/ nhess-16-977-2016

Lee, T. M., Markowitz, E. M., Howe, P. D., Ko, C.-Y., & Leiserowitz, A. A. (2015). Predictors of public climate change awareness and risk perception around the world. Nature Climate Change, 5(11), 11. https://doi.org/10.1038/ nclimate2728

Lenzholzer, S., Carsjens, G.-J., Brown, R. D., Tavares, S., Vanos, J., Kim, Y, & Lee, K. (2020). Urban climate awareness and urgency to adapt: An international overview. Urban Climate, 33, 100667. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.uclim.2020.100667

Linden, S. (2014). On the relationship between personal experience, affect and risk perception: The case of climate change. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(5), 430-440. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2008

OECD. (2021). OECD regional outlook 2021: Resilience in the COVID-19 crisis and transition to net zero greenhouse gas emissions. OECD. https://doi.org/10.1787/17017efe-en

OECD. (2023). Adaptation measurement: Assessing municipal climate risks to inform adaptation policy in the Slovak Republic. OECD. https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/content/paper/ dad34bb3-en

Poortinga, W., Whitmarsh, L., Steg, L., Bohm, G., & Fisher, S. (2019). Climate change perceptions and their individual-level determinants: A cross-European analysis. Global Environmental Change, 55, 25-35. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2019.01.007

Rosentrater, L. D., S^lensminde, I., Ekstrom, F., Bohm, G., Bostrom, A., Hanss, D., & O'Connor, R. E. (2013). Efficacy trade-offs in individuals' support for climate change policies. Environment and Behavior, 45(8), 935-970. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916512450510

Saari, U. A., Damberg, S., Frombling, L., & Ringle, C. M. (2021). Sustainable consumption behavior of Europeans: The influence of environmental knowledge and risk perception on environmental concern and behavioral intention. Ecological Economics, 189, 107155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2021.107155

Sanne, C. (2002). Willing consumers-Or locked-in? Policies for a sustainable consumption. Ecological Economics, 42(1-2), 273-287. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0921-8009(02)00086-1

Semenza, J. C., Ploubidis, G. B., & George, L. A. (2011). Climate change and climate variability: Personal motivation for adaptation and mitigation. Environmental Health, 10(1), 46. https://doi.org/10.1186/1476-069x-10-46

Shi, J., Visschers, V. H. M., & Siegrist, M. (2015). Public perception of climate change: The importance of knowledge and cultural worldviews: The importance of knowledge and cultural worldviews in climate change perception. Risk Analysis, 35(12), 2183-2201. https:// doi.org/10.1111/risa.12406

Statistical Office of the Slovak republic. (2022). The elections to the bodies of communal self-government 2022 [Data set]. Statistical Office of the Slovak republic. https://volby.statistics. sk/oso/oso2022/sk/subory_na_stiahnutie.html

Stern, P. C. (1992). Psychological dimensions of global environmental change. Annual Review of Psychology, 43(1), 269-302. https:// doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.43.020192.001413

Tapia, C., Abajo-Alda, B., Feliu, E., Mendizabal, M., Martinez-Saenz, J. A., Fernandez, G., Laburu, T., & Lejarazu, A. (2017). Profiling urban vulnerabilities to climate change: An indicator-based vulnerability assessment for European cities. Ecological Indicators, 78, 142-155. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.ecolind.2017.02.040

Tobler, C., Visschers, V. H. M., & Siegrist, M. (2012). Addressing climate change: Determinants of consumers' willingness to act and to support policy measures. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 32(3), 197-207. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.02.001

Valenzuela-Levi, N., Fuentes, L., Ramirez, M. I., Rodriguez, S., & Senoret, A. (2022). Urban sustainability and perceived satisfaction in neoliberal cities. Cities, 126, 103647. https://doi.org/10.10167j.cities.2022.103647

Veronika Toth (1), Miriam Sebova (2)

(1) Technical University of Kosice, Faculty of Economics, Department of Regional Sciences and Management, Slovakia, ORCID: 0000-0003-2136-8464, [email protected];

(2) Technical University of Kosice, Faculty of Economics, Department of Regional Sciences and Management, Slovakia, ORCID: 0000-0002-5157-5299, [email protected].

Caption: Fig. 1 Importance of variables in random forests models
Tab. 1: Descriptive statistics
Variable                          Min    Max    Mean    SD    Median
Age                               15.0   87.0   40.2   14.8    40.0
Income                            1.0    3.0    2.0    0.5     2.0
Education (years of schooling)    10.0   18.0   16.3   2.4     18.0
Sex (0 = male)                    0.0    1.0    0.6    0.5     1.0
Children (0 = no children)        0.0    1.0    0.6    0.5     1.0
Life satisfaction                 1.0    5.0    3.9    0.7     4.0
Interest in climate change        0.0    1.0    0.9    0.2     1.0
Subjective vulnerability          0.0    1.0    0.5    0.2     0.5
Citizen engagement                0.0    1.0    0.4    0.2     0.4
Information                       0.2    1.0    0.5    0.2     0.5
Engagement in climate-            0.0    1.0    0.6    0.2     0.7
friendly activities
Satisfaction with measures        0.0    1.0    0.4    0.2     0.4
Awareness                         0.1    1.0    0.8    0.2     0.8
Source: own
Tab. 2: Climate-related variables
Variable                    Type                     Description
Awareness         Average score from 2        (See below)
                  questions--knowledge and
                  seriousness (see below),
                  adjusted by min-max
                  normalization
Knowledge *       Index based on the summed   Statements concerning
                  scores from 10 statements   anthropogenic causes of
                  with Likert scale           climate change,
                  responses (5 levels),       scientific consensus on
                  adjusted by min-max         climate change, and
                  normalization               impacts of climate change
Seriousness *     Likert scale score (10      Degree of seriousness of
                  levels), divided by 10      climate change as an
                                              issue
Vulnerability     Index based on the summed   Statements concerning
                  scores from 8 statements    negative climate change
                  with Likert scale           impacts on health,
                  responses (5 levels),       agriculture,
                  adjusted by min-max         infrastructure and
                  normalization               property, services or
                                              business
Engagement        Index based on the summed   Examples of activities
                  scores from 8 examples      indicating engagement in
                  with Likert scale           local politics
                  responses (3 levels),
                  adjusted by min-max
                  normalization
Information       Average score from 2        Degree of satisfaction
                  questions with Likert       with available
                  scale responses (5          information concerning
                  levels), adjusted by        climate change impacts
                  min-max normalization       and adaptation options
Interest in       Dummy                       Interest in climate
climate change                                change
Satisfaction      Index based on the summed   Satisfaction with
with measures     scores from 15 examples     adaptation measures which
                  with Likert scale           are done by the local
                  responses (5 levels),       authorities, primarily in
                  adjusted by min-max         public spaces
                  normalization
Climate-          Index based on the summed   Examples of activities
friendly          scores from 17 examples     related to adaptation and
activities        (Tab. 4) with Likert        mitigation
                  scale responses (5
                  levels), adjusted by
                  min-max normalization
Variable             Questionnaire item
                   (example in the case of
                   multiple sub-questions)
Awareness         (See below)
Knowledge *       Research shows that
                  climate change is caused
                  by human activity
Seriousness *     How serious a problem do
                  you believe climate
                  change to be at the
                  moment?
Vulnerability     Summer heatwaves reduce
                  my work performance
                  (focus/attention)
Engagement        I participate in local
                  elections
Information       1) Do you feel
                  sufficiently informed
                  about climate change and
                  its impact on Kosice?
                  2) Do you feel
                  sufficiently informed
                  about how to adapt to
                  climate change in the
                  city?
Interest in       Are you interested in the
climate change    topic of climate change?
Satisfaction      E.g., air-conditioning in
with measures     public buildings
                  (including hospitals)
Climate-          See Tab. 4.
friendly
activities
Note: * variable not used directly in the model.
Source: own
Tab. 3: Climate-friendly activities
                                       Activity
Low-cost activities    I support the maintenance of green
                       spaces instead of new buildings and
                       parking spaces
                       I spend most of my time outside the city
                       (e.g., at a cottage, in the countryside)
                       during hot days
                       I compost my bio-degradable waste
                       Instead of a private car, I use
                       alternative means of transport which are
                       more sustainable for the environment
                       (walking, bicycle, public transport or
                       car-sharing)
                       I drink enough water, keep myself cool
                       and avoid direct sunlight during
                       heatwaves at noon
                       Whenever possible, I buy local products
                       and seasonal produce
                       I try to minimize my waste production,
                       e.g., by limiting the use of plastic
                       bags
                       I sort waste for recycling
                       I engage in volunteering activities
                       dedicated to the protection of the
                       environment
                       I follow extreme weather alerts
High-cost activities   I use air-conditioning during hot days
                       Low fuel consumption was an important
                       feature that I considered when buying a
                       new car
                       I changed my holiday plans due to
                       heatwaves (different dates or locations)
                       I installed additional shading equipment
                       at home
                       I insulated my house/apartment in order
                       to reduce temperate fluctuations
                       Whenever possible, I prefer to maintain
                       green spaces on my property
Source: own
Tab. 4: OLS results
                     Estimate, std. error and significance
Variables                Awareness          Activities
Intercept            0.484 (0.066) ***     0.103 (0.076)
Age                  -0.002 (0.001) **   0.002 (0.001) **
Income                0.026 (0.016).       0.017 (0.013)
Education              0.003 (0.002)       0.000 (0.003)
Sex (male)            -0.022 (0.013)     -0.033 (0.014) *
Children              -0.012 (0.016)       0.008 (0.017)
Life satisfaction     -0.006 (0.009)       0.014 (0.010)
Interest in          0.209 (0.030) ***     0.046 (0.037)
  climate change
Subjective           0.217 (0.039) ***   0.178 (0.038) ***
  vulnerability
Engagement            -0.007 (0.040)     0.264 (0.034) ***
Information           -0.001 (0.007)      0.017 (0.007) *
Activities             0.084 (0.058)
Satisfaction          -0.068 (0.044)      0.084 (0.050).
  with measures
Awareness                                  0.093 (0.064)
N                           545                 545
Adjusted R-squared         0.275               0.307
                     Estimate, std. error and significance
Variables            Activities (cheap)   Activities (expensive)
Intercept            0.278 (0.077) ***        -0.131 (0.098)
Age                    0.001 (0.001).        0.002 (0.001) **
Income                 -0.012 (0.013)        0.059 (0.019) **
Education              0.000 (0.003)          0.000 (0.004)
Sex (male)           -0.048 (0.013) ***       0.000 (0.018)
Children              -0.027 (0.016).        0.063 (0.023) **
Life satisfaction      0.011 (0.009)          0.014 (0.012)
Interest in            0.050 (0.038)          0.027 (0.040)
  climate change
Subjective            0.086 (0.036) *       0.286 (0.047) ***
  vulnerability
Engagement           0.289 (0.033) ***      0.156 (0.041) ***
Information           0.023 (0.007) **        0.018 (0.049)
Activities
Satisfaction           0.064 (0.051)          0.095 (0.058).
  with measures
Awareness              0.117 (0.063).         0.031 (0.063)
N                           545                    545
Adjusted R-squared         0.296                  0.199
Note: Significance codes: *** 0; ** 0.001; * 0.01; . 0.05.
Source: own


Veronika Toth ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2136-8464

Miriam Sebova ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5157-5299
COPYRIGHT 2024 Technical University of Liberec
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2024 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Article Details
Printer friendly Cite/link Email Feedback
Title Annotation:Economics; Kosice, Slovakia
Author:Toth, Veronika; Sebova, Miriam
Publication:E+M Ekonomie a Management
Article Type:Case study
Geographic Code:4EXSV
Date:Jan 1, 2024
Words:8518
Previous Article:The nexus of a regional competitiveness and economic resilience: The evidence-based on V4+4 NUTS 2 regions.
Next Article:Educational attainment as a predictor of poverty and social exclusion: Empirical analysis of Serbian case.
Topics:

Terms of use | Privacy policy | Copyright © 2024 Farlex, Inc. | Feedback | For webmasters |